Does psychosocial stress cause hypertension? A systematic review of observational studies

Share Embed


Descrição do Produto

Journal of Human Hypertension (2009) 23, 12–19 & 2009 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 0950-9240/09 $32.00 www.nature.com/jhh

REVIEW

Does psychosocial stress cause hypertension? A systematic review of observational studies F Sparrenberger1, FT Cichelero2, AM Ascoli2, FP Fonseca2, G Weiss2, O Berwanger3, SC Fuchs4, LB Moreira5 and FD Fuchs2 1

Department of Medicine, Universidade Regional de Blumenau, Blumenau, Brazil; 2Division of Cardiology, Hospital de Clı´nicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil; 3Hospital do Corac¸a˜o, Sa˜o Paulo, Brazil; 4Department of Social Medicine, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil and 5Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Hospital de Clı´nicas de Porto Alegre, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil

Acute stress promotes transient elevation of blood pressure, but there is no consistent evidence that this effect results in hypertension. In this systematic review of cohort and case–control studies that investigated the association between psychosocial stress and hypertension, we conducted a complete search up to February 2007 in MEDLINE, EMBASE, PSYCINFO and LILACS, through a search strategy that included eight terms to describe the exposure, six related to the design of the studies and one term for outcome. The quality was assessed by the Newcastle–Ottawa Quality Assessment Scale. The selection was done in duplicate by two teams of independent reviewers. Among 82 studies selected in the second phase, only 14 (10 cohort studies and 4 case–control studies), totalling 52 049 individuals, fulfilled the selection criteria. The average quality of

the studies was 6.6±1.3 in a 9-point scale. Acute life events were associated with hypertension in one and were not associated in two studies. Five out of seven studies found a significant and positive association between measures of chronic stress and hypertension, with risk ratios ranging from 0.8 to 11.1. Three out of five studies reported high and significant risks of affective response to stress for hypertension, one a significant risk close to a unit and one reported absence of risk. Acute stress is probably not a risk factor for hypertension. Chronic stress and particularly the non-adaptive response to stress are more likely causes of sustained elevation of blood pressure. Studies with better quality are warranted. Journal of Human Hypertension (2009) 23, 12–19; doi:10.1038/jhh.2008.74; published online 10 July 2008

Keywords: stress; psychological distress; life change events; hypertension; blood pressure; systematic review

Introduction The interaction between a genetic background and environmental and behavioural exposures, such as the excess of salt, fat and alcohol consumption, accounts for most but not all cases of hypertension. Stress has long been listed as a potential and important cause of hypertension1 among other potential risk factors such as low potassium consumption, low physical activity and sleep abnormalities. Acute stress can induce transient elevation of blood pressure, but it is still unclear whether this effect results in sustained elevation of blood pressure and hypertension.2 The results of the INTERHEART study, which identified an Correspondence: Dr FD Fuchs, Division of Cardiology, Hospital de Clı´nicas de Porto Alegre, UFRGS, Ramiro Barcelos, 2350, sala 2060, 90035-903, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil. E-mail: [email protected] Received 6 February 2008; revised 10 June 2008; accepted 12 June 2008; published online 10 July 2008

independent risk of stress for myocardial infarction,3 have renewed the interest on stress as a risk factor for cardiovascular diseases. Stress has been defined as a process in which environmental demands exceed the adaptive capacity of an organism, resulting in psychological and biological changes that may place persons at risk for disease.4 Both stressors and the adaptive response of individuals (the transaction between stressor and response) have been defined in different ways in experimental and epidemiological studies. In addition, epidemiological studies have different designs, follow-up times, sample criteria and variable definitions of hypertension. To our knowledge, there is no systematic review or meta-analysis of cohort and case–control studies focusing on stress as a risk factor for hypertension, probably because of the large variability of exposure and outcomes used in the original studies. There is only one systematic review of cohort studies showing that anxiety, depression, and anger control and social support

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al 13

were associated with the incidence of hypertension.5 The variability between studies precluded the undertaking of a meta-analysis. Therefore, we summarized the evidence about the association between stress and hypertension in observational studies through a systematic review, combining studies by design and type of stressors and adaptive response.

Methods Criteria for study inclusion

We restricted our analysis to cohort and case– control studies. In view of the paucity of longitudinal studies, case–control studies were required to increase the number of useful studies. Despite their higher propensity for bias, case–control studies assume that exposition occurred before the event. Cross-sectional studies were not included, as they are still more heterogeneous in terms of measures of stress and hypertension than the cohort and case– control studies. Further, causality cannot be inferred and there is currently no established method for the systematic review of such studies. The outcome of the studies should be the diagnosis of hypertension as a binary variable. Cases should be participants with hypertension and control individuals without such diagnosis. Participants were from both genders and at least 18 years of age. Cohort studies with participants with hypertension at the baseline evaluation were excluded. Studies that measured imposed physical–mental ‘loads’ or ensuing pathophysiological states6 were included in the process of study selection. Experimental studies, demographic studies and studies of personality and coping (set of strategies used by individuals to adapt themselves to adverse or stressful events) were not included. Studies that included participants with psychiatric diagnoses, such as anxiety and depression, were also excluded. Search strategy

We conducted a complete search up to February 2007 in MEDLINE, EMBASE, PSYCINFO and LILACS, through search strategy and terms (full-text and keywords) presented in the panel. The search terms were selected from the MeSH Database, selecting 5 of 95 terms related to stress, and also terms suggested by MeSH (‘see also’). Additionally, we searched the references of the articles selected in the databases. We did not apply any restriction in regard to the language of the publication and did not use any strategy of search for studies that were not published.

were excluded at this step, as they were not clearly a cohort or case–control studies or they provided other evidence of inadequacy with regard to the selection criteria. In phase II, the selected articles were evaluated in their full-text format to decide whether they fulfilled the criteria of eligibility. The following information was extracted from the studies that fulfilled the eligibility criteria: first author, country and year of publication, age composition, sample size, criteria of definition of exposure and outcome, duration of follow-up (cohort), number of events according to the factor in study, variables controlled in the multivariate analysis and effect measures (relative risk, odds ratio (OR) or hazards ratio). In view of the variable definition of exposure both in terms of stressors and adaptive response, we aggregated the studies by design, acute and chronic nature of exposure to stressors, and by studies that evaluated the affective response. Studies of affective response were defined on the basis of use of any questionnaire oriented to the perception of stress. The studies used quite variable instruments, but mostly rating scales. Hypertension was defined by the authors of the studies, and when more than one criterion was used we chose blood pressure X140/90 mm Hg or use of blood pressure drugs. Appraisal of the quality of studies

We used the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale to assess the quality of studies.7 A score of quality was calculated on the basis of three major components: selection of the groups of study (0–4 points), quality of the adjustment for confounding (0–2 points) and ascertainment of the exposure or outcome of interest in the case–control or cohorts, respectively (0–3 points). The maximum score would be 9 points, representing the highest methodological quality (Table 1). Adjustment for baseline blood pressure values, duration of follow-up of at least 5 years and losses that did not exceed 10% for cohort studies, and adjustment for age and non-response rates below 10% in case–control studies were criteria of higher quality. In addition, we employed funnel plots to examine the possibility of publication bias separately for the different definitions of stressors and adaptive responses. Extraction of data and appraisal of the quality of studies were carried out in duplicate by two teams of independent reviewers (GW and FPF, FTC and AMA), who were previously trained and certified. The first author resolved the divergences between the reviewers.

Results Extraction of data

In phase I, the titles and abstracts resultant of the search strategy was evaluated to select studies with potential to fulfil the inclusion criteria. Most studies

The initial search in abstracts identified 593 articles that were potentially eligible. The large number of studies selected in the first phase was explained by the ease of primary selection, as stress and Journal of Human Hypertension

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al 14

Table 1 Appraisal of methodological quality (Newcastle–Ottawa

Description of the studies

Scale)

Ten studies were cohorts and four case–controls, totalling 52 049 individuals. The study of Radi et al.8 reported two different forms of exposures and was analysed separately. Details of studies are presented in Table 2. The largest cohort, with 30 330 black women and 2316 cases of hypertension, included more participants than the sum of the remaining studies.21 Cohort studies totalled 50 471 individuals and identified 7800 cases of incident hypertension (15.5%). Nine different definitions of outcome were employed and only three8,10,16 used the most contemporaneous definition of hypertension (blood pressure X140/90 mm Hg or use of blood pressurelowering agents). All studies were adjusted for age and for a variable set of confounders, including body mass index in nine and previous blood pressure levels and education in five. The mean age of participants at entry also varied, but most studies were carried out with young or middle-age adults. The study by Levenstein et al.15 included only adults, and the study by Sparrow et al.19 did not present a measure of central tendency. The study by Markovitz et al.18 supplied information on the racial distribution of the sample, which included 45.2% of black individuals, and the study by Cozier et al. was restricted to black women. The duration of follow-up of cohorts varied from 2.5 to 20 years.

Case—control Acute Radi8 Chronic Schnall9 Radi8

Selection

Comparability

Exposure

Total

%%%

%%

%

6

%%%% %%%

%% %%

%% %

8 6

%% %%

%%% %

8 6

Affective response Perez10 %%% %%% El-Shafei11 Cohort

Selection

Comparability

Outcome

Acute Perini12 Dorn13

%%% %%%%

%% %

% %%

6 7

Chronic Kahn14 Levenstein15 Nakanishi16 Fauvel17 Markovitz18

%%%% %%% %%% %%% %%%

% % %% %% %%

%% % %%% %% %%

7 5 8 7 7

Affective response Sparrow19 %%% %%%% Everson20 %% Cozier21

%% %% %

%% %

7 7 3

Methodological quality of the studies

Figure 1 Flow chart of the selection of studies.

hypertension are very common terms. Eighty-two studies fulfilled the criteria of eligibility for the first phase and were fully evaluated. Seventy-three studies were excluded in the second phase, mostly because they did not have exposures or outcomes defined as required for this review (Figure 1). The list of these articles may be requested from the correspondent author. On the basis of the list of references of the nine studies, it was possible to identify five others that fulfilled the criteria of eligibility and were therefore included in the analysis. Journal of Human Hypertension

The quality of the studies varied from 3 to 8 points, with a mean of 6.6±1.3 and a median of 7 (Table 1). The studies with higher scores10,16 did not get a score of 9 because their samples were not representative of communities. Nakanishi et al.16 derived their cohort from the workers of only one company, and Perez et al.10 informed that their cases had originally come from the health services of Yarumal, Colombia, but did not inform whether they were representative of the individuals who needed medical care. The study with the lower score was exactly the largest one.21 The authors derived their sample from subscribers of a magazine; exposures and outcome were self-reported, and the adjustment was made only for reported age and body mass index, besides having a short time of follow-up and high proportion of losses (26%). The funnel plot for acute and chronic studies was unequivocally symmetric, suggesting that publication bias was unlikely (Figure 2a). For studies of affective response, the funnel was not so evident, but there were fewer studies (Figure 2b). Association between different definitions of stress and hypertension

The studies were grouped by design (cohort or case–control study) and definition of stress (acute, chronic or affective response to stress). All studies of

Table 2 Characteristics of the studies included in the review First author, year of publication, country

Age at entry (years)

Exposure

9586 (0)

440

Sparrow, 1982, USA19

1166 (0)

23–80

Perini, 1991, Switzerland12

121 (41)

16–24

Chronic stressors (29 single items) Affective response (19 score items grouping in 5 items) Life events (14 score items)

Everson, 2000, Finland20

616 (0)

50.4±6.6

Hopelessness (2 score items)

Levenstein, 2001, USA15

2357 (56)

Adults

Job (2 single items), social alienation

Nakanishi, 2001, Japan16

941 (0)

35–54

Fauvel, 2003, France20

274 (8)

Markovitz, 2004, USA18

Cohort Kahn, 1972, Israel14

Follow-up (years)

No events/ cases

Definition of outcome

Adjustment

5

3829

BP X160/95 mm Hg

Age, place of birth

10

64

BP X160/95 mm Hg

2.5

5

BP X160/95 mm Hg

4.2

126

BP X165/95 mm Hg or use of BP drugs

20

637

Use of BP drugs self-reported

Work hours

5

336

BP X140/90 mm Hg or use of BP drugs

36±1

Job content questionnaire

5

93

3200 (55)

20–32

Job content questionnaire

8

89

Cozier, 2006, USA21

30 330 (100)

37 (MED)

4

2316

Dorn, 2007, Netherlands13

1880 (52.3)

46.3

Perceived racism (8 single items) Life events (caregivers of fire victims)

D SBP/DBP 47 mm Hg or DBP 495 mm Hg BP X160/95 mm Hg or use of BP drugs Self-reported

BP initial, age, haematocrit, skin fold sub scapular, BMI BP initial, CVR, plasma nor adrenaline, psychological factors, parental hypertension, weight, sodium clearance Age, BMI, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, education, SBP initial, parental hypertension, depressive symptoms Age, medical check-up, BMI, smoking, physical activity, alcohol, education, race, unemployment, depression Age, BP initial, BMI, alcohol, smoking, fruits and vegetable, physical activity, salt, sleep hours, breakfast Sex, age, BP initial, HR at entry, CVR, alcohol, sodium clearance

4

305

Medical diagnosis

Age, sex, insurance type, history of chronic disease, number of medical consultation, single parenthood

196 (0)

45±8.7

Job content questionnaire

76

DBP 485 mm Hg

Perez, 2001, Colombia10

456 (64)

54±12.4

Structured questionnaire



87

BP X140/90 mm Hg or use of BP drugs

El-Shafei, 2002, Egypt11

361 (52.1)

75.8% 440



165

Medical diagnosis



203



203

BP X140/90 mm Hg or use of BP drugs BP X140/90 mm Hg or use of BP drugs

Age, BMI, type A, alcohol, education, smoking Age, sex, obesity, physical activity, salt, alcohol, smoking, fat, education, stroke FH heart failure and AMI BMI, age, sex, occupation, smoking, diet Age, education, behaviour, social support Age, education, behaviour, social support

Case–control Schnall, 1990, USA9

Radi, 2005, France

565 (30)

Radi, 2005, France8

565(30)

Semistructured questionnaire 41.8±7.8 men, Life stress (14 score items 43.5±7.5 women dichotomized) 41.8±7.8 men, Job content questionnaire 43.5±7.5 women

Age, BMI, education, SBP initial Age, BMI, questionnaire cycle

Abbreviations: AMI, acute myocardial infarction; BMI, body mass index; BP, blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; FH, familiar history; HR, heart rate; MED, median; CVR, cardiovascular reactivity; SBP, systolic blood pressure.

15

Journal of Human Hypertension

8

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al

Sample size (% women)

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al 16 12000

10000

10000 Sample size

Sample size

Affective response 12000

8000 6000 4000

8000 6000 4000 2000

2000

0

0 0

4

2

Effect size

6

8

0

2

4 6 Effect size

8

Figure 2 Funnel plots for studies of acute and chronic stress (a) and for studies of affective response (b).

chronic stress were occupational. The case–control study by Radi et al.8 included life events and occupational stress. When more than one risk ratio was presented, the higher and statistically significant estimate was chosen. The risk ratios for hypertension and the corresponding confidence intervals, when available, are presented in Figure 3. Acute stress

The three studies, two cohorts and one case–control, which assessed the association between acute stress and hypertension reported null, inverse or positive association. Perini et al.12 evaluated a list of 14 events as a continuous variable. Radi et al.8 defined exposure by the report of any among a list of 14 events. Dorn et al.13 measured exposure to a disaster (being the parent of an adolescent involved in a mass burn incident) and found a direct association. After this episode, the victims’ parents were forced to deal with a great number of stressful experiences in their function as caregivers, such as physical disabilities and emotional scars. Chronic stress

Four studies8,9,18,17 used the Job Content Questionnaire,22 an instrument to measure occupational stress, focusing on the forms of work organization. Fauvel et al.17 reported an inverse association and the remaining studies identified a statistically significant association between stress and hypertension. Three studies used singular items of exposure. Kahn et al.14 investigated a list of 90 exposures, 30 being items of current chronic stressors. Only two of these, both occupational items, were associated with hypertension. Levenstein et al.15 tested only two indicators of stress on the job, describing a risk for ‘worried about keeping the job’ and null association for ‘average or not good at doing the job’. Nakanishi et al.16 reported an inverse relation between working hours and hypertension. Four studies exclusively studied employees. Two of them8,9 had selected workers of diverse companies and the other two, those of a single company.16,17 Journal of Human Hypertension

Three out of the five studies included10,11,20 demonstrated a statistically significant association between stress, measured by different scales, and hypertension. Everson et al.,20 El-Shafei et al.11 and Perez et al.10 identified risk ratios for hypertension between 1.6 and 33.7 for hopelessness, severe stress and ‘tension-anxiety’. Sparrow et al.19 derived five measures from stress from the Medical Cornell Index and did not identify an association between stress and hypertension. In the same way, Cozier et al.21 adapted one instrument of perceived racism, composed for eight items, and found a few risk ratios for hypertension but very close to null.

Discussion This qualitative systematic review tried to include the aggregate of all observational evidences with a cohort and case–control design that had evaluated the association between stress and hypertension. The number of studies selected in the first phase of the searching strategy was impressive, but just a few studies fulfilled the criteria of selection. Even for this small number of studies, the quality and number of individuals investigated were not satisfactory; for instance, the largest study21 had the lowest score of quality. Moreover, stressors and adaptive responses were quite variable in the studies. It was therefore impossible to summarize the evidence for the association between a unique and well-defined characterization of exposure to stressors and the subsequent adaptive response and the incidence of hypertension. We then aggregated the studies by design and main exposures, looking at the trends for risk in each situation. Overall, only when the studies explored the affective response to stressors was there a consistent risk for hypertension. The association between acute stress and hypertension was evaluated in only three studies,8,12,13 which had inconsistent results. The case–control study showed an inverse association in men and women,8 but the probability of a recall bias cannot be ruled out. One of the cohort studies showed a null association12 and the other a positive association.13 From the studies of chronic stress, five reported at least one significant risk for hypertension among several measures of stress,8,9,14,15,18 and two were negative.16,17 The positive studies had a mean average score of quality of 6.6, in comparison with a score of 7.5 for the two studies with negative findings. The distinction of acute and chronic stress was in some way arbitrary, as some acute events have long-lasting repercussion. An example is the study by Dorn et al.,13 who studied parents of adolescent fire victims. This acute stressful event definitely has a long-lasting duration. On the other hand, studies classified primarily by the presence of chronic stressors had repetitive and recurrent exposition to stressors, particularly on the job.

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al 17

Figure 3 Odds ratio (OR) for hypertension by different criteria of the definition of stress: a summary of case–control and cohort studies; studies presenting only the point estimate did not have confidence intervals reported by the authors; the ORs for the studies of Perini et al. and Sparrow et al. were assumed as one, as the authors reported just the absence of a significant association.

Taken together, the lack of consistent findings in studies with acute stress events and hypertension and the trend for association in those with chronic stress exposure suggests that only persistent and repetitive stressors would have the potential to induce sustained elevation of blood pressure.23 Moreover, the number of acute stressful events reported in the studies of Radi et al.8 and Perini et al.12 may be insufficient, as a range of 30–50 items seems both optimal in terms of predictive power and efficient in terms of use of time and space.23 From the studies that used the Job Content Questionnaire,8,9,18,17 only Fauvel et al.17 did not find a positive association. In this study, the cumulative incidence of hypertension seems to be even higher among those not exposed to stress. A limitation of all studies, but not of the study of Markovitz et al.,18 is that their authors assumed that exposure had not varied along the follow-up. Markovitz et al.12 applied the questionnaire at the start and end of the follow-up and found an OR of 2.06 (confidence interval (CI) 95% 1.01–4.26) for the increase in the ratio job demand/decision latitude. Another potential limitation of the cohort studies

was the losses in follow-up, which varied from 0 to 68%. Studies that presented original analysis had fewer losses,12,13,14,16,17 which were higher in reanalysis, mostly because of incomplete data.8,15,18–20 There was no trend for an association between the proportion of losses and the estimates of risk. Moreover, there is no evident reason to suppose that the losses had any systematic direction. Studies of affective response seem to be more appropriate to investigate the association between stress events and hypertension. They integrate both the stressor events, acute or chronic, the response to them and characteristics of the individuals, social support and other known and unknown mechanisms that could link stress to hypertension.24 A large number of instruments to measure the affective response have been presented in the literature. From the five studies that addressed this exposure and fulfilled the criteria of inclusion in this systematic review, one19 did not find an association between the affective response to stress and hypertension, and three10,11,20 reported positive association and a trend for a dose–response effect. The ORs were 1.27 (95% CI 0.79–2.07),16 2.85 (95% CI 1.59–5.11)14 and Journal of Human Hypertension

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al 18

8.70 (95% CI 2.61–28.86)20 for moderate stress and OR 3.22 (95% CI 1.56–6.67),16 5.02 (95% CI 2.25–11.19)14 and 10.10 (95% CI 3.02–33.72)20 for high stress. The study by Cozier et al.21 had a low score of quality and explored the association of a large number of exposures with self-reported hypertension. These findings are in accordance with the results of laboratory experiments. It has been suggested that individuals who presented a more intense haemodynamic response to stressful tasks have a familial predisposition to hypertension.25 Impaired cardiovascular recovery following stress seems to predict the risk of developing hypertension.26 High cardiovascular reactivity to stress would depend on an interaction between genetic factors (familial history) and environment (frequency of exposure to stressors).27 The response to exposures could be influenced by former experiences.28 These sympatheticinduced haemodynamic responses to stressful events29 could lead to endothelial dysfunction and proliferation of smooth muscular cells,30 perpetuating the hypertensive state. The investigation of the link between stress events, affective response and hypertension would require further studies in terms of frequency and magnitude of exposure and duration of the cardiovascular recovery after stress. Beyond the direct physiopathological consequences of stress, it could be associated with unhealthy habits such as dietary habits, excessive alcoholic beverages consumption and low levels of physical activity.29 Clinical trials demonstrating that measures to control stress are effective in lowering blood pressure would be helpful in confirming the link between stress and hypertension. One protocol for a systematic review of five clinical trials was registered with the Cochrane Library, focusing on the effect of relaxation in the treatment of patients with essential hypertension.31 Anxiety and depression could be part of the maladaptive response to stressors, and therefore would be in the chain of causation of hypertension and other illnesses. These conditions are psychiatric conditions that require formal criteria for diagnosis, and could occur independently of identifiable triggering factors, such as environmental stressors. A quantitative review identified that psychological factors could be associate with hypertension,5 but the isolate contribution of each component has been questioned.32 To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review addressing the association between stress and hypertension in cohort and case–control studies. We had planned to undertake a meta-analysis, but the heterogeneity between the studies, with regard to design, definition of exposure and outcome and control for confounding, excluded this possibility. The search for studies was conducted in four different databases and references of the studies. The use of methodological filters of observational studies increased the specificity of title Journal of Human Hypertension

and abstract resultant of the search strategy. The use of two teams of independent reviewers probably enhanced the quality of study selection and evaluation. The main limitation of our systematic review was the absence of an attempt to identify unpublished investigations, which could have biased our findings as a consequence of a publication bias. The small number of studies that fulfilled our eligibility criteria, however, suggests that it is unlikely that a properly conducted study did not report its findings. In conclusion, the few cohort and case–control studies addressing the association between stress and the incidence of hypertension used variable definitions of stressors and the adaptive response to them. In addition, some of these studies had low methodological quality. With the aggregation of studies by exposure and response, it is possible to conclude that acute stressful events have no consistent association with incident hypertension. Chronic stress and particularly the non-adaptive response to stress are more likely causes of sustained elevation of blood pressure. Panel: search strategy and terms to identify articles (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) (15) (16) (17) (18)

‘Life change events’ ‘Stress’ ‘General adaptation syndrome’ ‘Stress disorders, post-traumatic’ ‘Combat disorders’ ‘Stress, psychological’ ‘Stress disorders, traumatic’ ‘Stress disorders, traumatic acute’ (1) OR (2) OR (3) OR (4) OR (5) OR (6) OR (7) OR (8) ‘Hypertension’ ‘Follow-up’ ‘Longitudinal’ ‘Prospective’ ‘Retrospective’ ‘Case–control’ ‘Cohort’ (11) OR (12) OR (13) OR (14) OR (15) OR (16) (9) AND (10) AND (17)

Acknowledgements This study was funded by grants from CNPq, CAPES and FAPERGS.

References 1 Heine H, Weiss M. Life stress and hypertension. Eur Heart J 1987; 8(Suppl B): 45–55. 2 Unger T, Parati G. Acute stress and long-lasting blood pressure elevation: a possible cause of established hypertension? J Hypertension 2005; 23: 261–263. ˆ unpuu S, Sliwa K, Zubaid 3 Rosengren A, Hawken S, O M, Almahmeed WA et al. Association of psychosocial risk factors with risk of acute myocardial infarction in 11119 cases and 13648 controls from 52 countries (the INTERHEART study): case–control study. Lancet 2004; 364: 953–962.

Stress and hypertension F Sparrenberger et al 19

4 Cohen S, Kessler RC, Underwood-Gordon L. Strategies for measuring stress in studies of psychiatric and physical disorders. In: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Underwood-Gordon L (eds). Measuring Stress. Oxford University Press: New York, 1995, pp 3–28. 5 Rutledge T, Hogan BE. A quantitative review of prospective evidence linking psychological factors with hypertension development. Psychosom Med 2002; 64: 758–766. 6 Folkow B. Mental stress and its importance for cardiovascular disorders; physiological aspects, ‘frommice-to-man’. Scand Cardiovasc J 2001; 35: 163–172. 7 Wells GA, Shea A, O’Connell D, Peterson J, Welch J, Losos M et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analyses (Homepage on the Internet). Ottawa Health Research Institute: Ottawa, c1996–2006 [updated 09 June 2007; cited 06 September 2007]. Available from: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_ epidemiology/oxford.htm. 8 Radi S, Lang T, Lauwers-Cance`s V, Die`ne E, Chatellier G, Larabi L et al. Job constraints and arterial hypertension: different effects in men and women: the IHPAF II case–control study. Occup Environ Med 2005; 62: 711–717. 9 Schnall PL, Landsbergis PA, Baker D. The relationship between ‘job strain,’ workplace diastolic blood pressure and left ventricular mass index. Results of a case–control study. JAMA 1990; 263: 1929–1935. 10 Perez LH, Gutierrez LA, Vioque J, Torres Y. Relation between overweight, diabetes, stress and hypertension: a case-control study in Yarumal – Antioqia, Colombia. Eur J Epidemiol 2001; 17: 275–280. 11 El-Shafei SA, Bassili A, Hassanien NM, Mokhtar MM. Genetic determinants of essential hypertension. J Egypt Public Health Assoc 2002; 77: 231–246. 12 Perini C, Mu¨ller FB, Bu¨hler FR. Suppressed aggression accelerates early development of essential hypertension. J Hypertension 1991; 9: 499–503. 13 Dorn T, Yzermans CJ, Guijt H, Zee J. Disaster-related stress as a prospective risk factor for hypertension in parents of adolescent fire victims. Am J Epidemiol 2007; 165: 410–417. 14 Kahn HA, Medalie JH, Neufeld HN, Riss E, Goldbourt U. The incidence of hypertension and associated factors: the Israel Ischemic Disease Study. Am Heart J 1972; 8: 171–182. 15 Levenstein S, Smith MW, Kaplan GA. Psychosocial predictors of hypertension in men and women. Arch Intern Med 2001; 161: 1341–1346. 16 Nakanishi N, Yoshida H, Nagano K, Kawashimo H, Nakamura K, Tatara K. Long working hours and risk for hypertension in Japanese male white collar workers. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001; 55: 316–322. 17 Fauvel JP, Mpio I, Quelin P, Rigaud JP, Laville M, Ducher M. Neither perceived job stress nor individual cardiovascular reactivity predict high blood pressure. Hypertension 2003; 42: 1112–1116.

18 Markovitz JH, Matthews KA, Whooley M, Lewis CE, Greenlund KJ. Increases in job strain are associated with incident hypertension in the CARDIA Study. Ann Behav Med 2004; 28: 4–9. 19 Sparrow D, Garvey AJ, Rosner B, Thomas HE. Factors predicting blood pressure change. Circulation 1982; 65: 789–794. 20 Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Salonen JT. Hypertension incidence is predicted by high levels of hopelessness in finish men. Hypertension 2000; 35: 561–567. 21 Cozier Y, Palmer JR, Horton NJ, Fredman L, Wise LA, Rosenberg L. Racial discrimination and the incidence of hypertension in US black women. Ann Epidemiol 2006; 16: 681–687. 22 Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy Work: Stress, Productivity and the Reconstruction of Working Life. Basic Books: New York, 1990. 23 Turner J, Wheaton B. Checklist measurement of stressful life events. In: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU (eds). Measuring stress. A Guide for Health and Social Scientists. Oxford University Press: New York, 1997, pp 29–58. 24 Stone AA. Measurement of affective response. In: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU (eds). Measuring Stress. A Guide for Health and Social Scientists. Oxford University Press: New York, 1997, pp 148–171. 25 Light KC, Girdler SS, Sherwood A, Bragdon EE, Brownley KA, West SG et al. High stress responsivity predicts later blood pressure only in combination with positive family history and high life stress. Hypertension 1999; 33: 1458–1464. 26 Steptoe A, Marmot M. Impaired cardiovascular recovery following stress predicts 3-year increases in blood pressure. J Hypertens 2005; 23: 529–536. 27 Light KC. Hypertension and the reactivity hypothesis: the next generation. Psychosom Med 2003; 63: 744–746. 28 Carroll D, Philips AC, Ring C, Der G, Hunt K. Life events and hemodynamic stress reactivity in the middle-aged and elderly. Psychophysiology 2005; 42: 269–276. 29 Schwartz AR, Gerin W, Davidson KW, Pickering TG, Brosschot JF, Thayer JF et al. Toward causal model of cardiovascular responses to stress and the development of cardiovascular disease. Psychosom Med 2003; 65: 22–35. 30 Markovitz JH, Jonas BS, Davidson K. Psychological factors as precursors to hypertension. Curr Hypertens Rep 2001; 3: 25–32. 31 Nicolson DJ, Dickinson HO, Campbell F, Cook J, Renton F, Ford GA et al. Relaxation therapies for the management of essential hypertension in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2004; (3): CD004935. 32 Wiehe M, Fuchs SC, Moreira LB, Moraes RS, Pereira GM, Gus M et al. Absence of association between depression and hypertension: results of a prospectively designed population-based study. J Hum Hypertens 2006; 20: 434–439.

Journal of Human Hypertension

Lihat lebih banyak...

Comentários

Copyright © 2017 DADOSPDF Inc.