Episodic memory in frontotemporal dementia: a critical review

Share Embed


Descrição do Produto

doi:10.1093/brain/aws011

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

| 678

BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY

REVIEW ARTICLE

Episodic memory in frontotemporal dementia: a critical review Michael Hornberger1,2 and Olivier Piguet1,2

Correspondence to: Olivier Piguet, Neuroscience Research Australia, Barker St, Randwick NSW 2031, Australia E-mail: [email protected]

This review offers a critical appraisal of the literature on episodic memory performance in frontotemporal dementia. Historically, description of patients diagnosed with what was then known as Pick’s disease included the presence of memory deficits and an underlying amnestic syndrome was noted in some of these patients. Over the last 20 years, however, the clinical view has been that episodic memory processing is relatively intact in the frontotemporal dementia syndrome. In particular, patients with the subtypes of behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and progressive non-fluent aphasia are reported to perform within normal limits on standard memory tests. In the third clinical presentation of frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, relatively intact episodic memory against a significantly impaired semantic memory was regarded as the hallmark. This position was instrumental in the development of clinical diagnostic criteria for frontotemporal dementia in which amnesia was explicitly listed as an exclusion criterion for the disease. The relative intactness of episodic memory, therefore, appeared to be a useful diagnostic marker to distinguish early frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease, in which early episodic memory disturbance remains the most common clinical feature. We argue that recent evidence questions the validity of preserved episodic memory in frontotemporal dementia, particularly in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. In semantic dementia, a complex picture emerges with preservation of some components of episodic memory, notably recognition-based visual memory and recall of recent autobiographical events. We propose a critical synthesis of recent neuropsychological evidence on retrograde and anterograde memory in light of neuroimaging and neuropathological findings, demonstrating involvement of medial temporal structures in frontotemporal dementia, structures known to be critical for episodic memory processing. We further argue that the multifactorial nature of most memory tests commonly used clinically fail to capture the memory deficits in frontotemporal dementia and that sensitive assessment tools of memory are needed. Together, recent clinical and experimental findings and the historical evidence represent a strong case for a re-evaluation of the importance of memory disturbance in the clinical diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia.

Keywords: anterograde memory; retrograde memory; semantic dementia Abbreviations: FTD = frontotemporal dementia; PNFA = progressive non-fluent aphasia

Received June 29, 2011. Revised November 10, 2011. Accepted December 19, 2011 ß The Author (2012). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected]

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

1 Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, NSW 2031, Australia 2 School of Medical Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2051, Australia

Episodic memory in FTD

Introduction

Historical evidence Arnold Pick: original cases Between 1892 and 1904, Arnold Pick, a neurologist in Prague, meticulously described five patients with distinct language changes (Pick, 1892, 1901, 1904). The clinical presentation was characterized by loss of word despite preserved knowledge of action and usage and was labelled amnestic aphasia. Other language disturbances included impaired repetition, phonemic substitution and neologisms. Significant focal brain atrophy was present in all cases at post-mortem, being most pronounced in the left temporal region with involvement of the frontal regions. Pick’s reports demonstrated that specific language symptoms could result from focal degeneration rather than diffuse and generalized atrophy as previously believed. Despite an emphasis on language and behavioural changes, episodic memory disturbance was reported in three

| 679

of these five patients. In particular, one patient, Josefa V., was described to have a ‘striking loss of memory’ with geographical and time disorientation and disturbance of autobiographical information. In other words, although not a central feature of the clinical phenomenology of this disease, evidence of anterograde and retrograde episodic memory disturbance was present in one, possibly three, of the five cases reported by Pick.

1920–1950: early case series of Pick’s disease The next 50 years saw the publication of small series of clinical cases of Pick’s disease with or without pathological confirmation, with a majority of clinical descriptions focusing on language disturbance. Two types of Pick’s disease were described: with or without aphasia (Kahn and Thompson, 1934) and with the presence of behavioural changes (e.g. a loss of high-level social graces and behaviours). The presentation of Pick’s disease was reported to be uniform and predictable (‘monotonie de la pre´sentation’) in contrast to Alzheimer’s disease (Caron, 1934). In these case series, memory was generally reported to be impaired due to faulty retrieval strategies resulting in inefficient memory processes (i.e. ‘loss of memory as a tool’) (Kahn and Thompson, 1934) or a lack of attention (Schneider, 1929). Memory deficits could be present in later stages of the disease due to developing dementia (Caron, 1934), although some disputed the presence of true amnesic deficits in Pick’s disease (Nichols and Weigner, 1938). For these authors, the preserved memory was one of the chief features distinguishing Pick’s disease from other dementias, including Alzheimer’s disease. Concurrently, evidence of anterograde memory deficit in cases of Pick’s disease with pathological confirmation emerged (Lo¨wenberg, 1936; Urechia, 1940). Such anterograde memory deficits took the form of impaired memory for recent events, disorientation in time, with increased apathy and indifference (Urechia, 1940), preserved long-term autobiographical memory and absence of language disturbance at presentation (Lo¨wenberg, 1936). Post-mortem investigations revealed significant anterior frontal and temporal atrophy that was asymmetric. Microscopic investigations revealed neuronal loss, spongiosis and the presence of Pick bodies and argentophilic inclusions, involving subregions of the hippocampus. Importantly, none of these cases showed Alzheimer-related pathology (plaques or tangles).

1950–1975: systematic investigations The 1950s see a more systematic approach to Pick’s disease: van Mansvelt (1954) published the first comprehensive and systematic clinicopathological review of all known cases of Pick’s disease. This exhaustive and painstaking work examined the clinical features and pathological findings of 196 cases. Disorientation was reported in 64 cases (33%) accompanied by loss of memory in a majority of cases. For van Mansvelt, however, ‘the disturbances of memory are not the result of a loss in the function of memory (in the sense of imprinting, retention and reproduction), but a result of an incapacity to order

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a progressive neurodegenerative brain disorder characterized by deterioration of behaviour, personality and language abilities in association with prominent frontal and temporal lobar atrophy. Three major clinical variants are recognized: behavioural variant FTD, semantic dementia and progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA) (Neary et al., 1998; Hodges and Miller, 2001). Neglected for over half of the 20th century following the original descriptions of the condition by Arnold Pick in the late 1890s and early 1900s, FTD has received renewed interest in the past 30 years. This rediscovery was stimulated in part by the increased awareness of patients presenting with focal neural degeneration associated with circumscribed cognitive deficits and behavioural changes, at least in the early disease stages of the disease. Investigations of patients with FTD have therefore provided invaluable insights into the nature of psychological processes such as language, semantic knowledge, emotion processing and social behaviour. This review focuses on the cognitive processes related to episodic memory in FTD, which have been systematically investigated only recently. One reason for this neglect is that impaired episodic memory performance has been considered an exclusion criterion for a clinical diagnosis of FTD, more particularly behavioural variant FTD. We will argue that the notion of absence of episodic memory deficits in FTD is incorrect. Episodic memory deficits were documented in the historical reports and recent findings on episodic memory function in FTD reinforce this position. As such, the status of episodic memory in FTD deserves to be revisited. We will also argue that appraisal of the current literature on episodic memory in FTD has theoretical and clinical implications, in particular for distinguishing patients with early Alzheimer’s disease from FTD, a distinction which is largely based on the difference in episodic memory performance. We will first review the historical evidence for episodic memory performance in FTD before discussing contemporaneous findings.

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

680

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

deficit was typically accompanied by severe atrophy of the frontal convexity. The authors finally note that 5 of their 32 cases did not show any memory disturbance, even in an advanced stage. Taken together, the historical literature demonstrates that disturbance of episodic memory is an early clinical feature in a portion of patients with Pick’s disease. This memory deficit was reported in the very first cases described by Pick. Undoubtedly, memory deficits were less common than behavioural or language changes and, as indicated by Tissot and colleagues (1975), could be masked by other, more prominent, clinical features. In some series, memory deficit was not reported, potentially not recognized, or, in some instances, possibly even ignored (e.g. Caron, 1934). Pathologically, the presence of neuropathological changes (neuronal loss, intraneuronal deposition) in brain structures known to play a critical role in episodic formation and retrieval also provides strong support for the presence of episodic memory deficit in this disease. Importantly, the evidence reviewed suggests that the pathological evidence was not always taken into account. In the subsequent years, further systematic investigations got under way, led by two research groups in Lund, Sweden and Manchester, UK (Brun, 1987; Neary et al., 1988). This research endeavoured to define the clinical characteristics of this non-Alzheimer disease pathology. This research paved the way for what is now known clinically as FTD and pathologically referred to as frontotemporal lobar degeneration. One major outcome was the publication of clinical diagnostic criteria, which for the first time proposed sets of guidelines that would help with the diagnosis of the main subtypes of FTD: behavioural variant FTD or simply FTD, semantic dementia and PNFA (Brun et al., 1994; Neary et al., 1998). At that time, the prominence of Alzheimer’s disease in dementia research, and the high prevalence of that disease compared with that of FTD, probably played a role in defining episodic memory deficit as an exclusion criterion for a diagnosis of FTD; in other words, defining FTD as not Alzheimer’s disease. These clinical diagnostic criteria guidelines have been recently revisited in two separate publications, the first focussing on behavioural variant FTD (Rascovsky et al., 2011) and the second proposing a classification of the language presentations of FTD within the umbrella of primary progressive aphasias (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011). The neuropathological classification of FTD or frontotemporal lobar degeneration has also evolved considerably since the first description by Pick, but remains heterogeneous (Mackenzie et al., 2010). About 40% of cases with FTD exhibit the microtubule-binding protein tau inclusions. Most remaining cases are tau-negative but show the 43 kDa TAR DNA-binding protein (TDP-43) inclusions. Recently, a small proportion of cases that are negative for both tau and TDP-43 have been found to show the RNA-binding protein fused in sarcoma (FUS) inclusions (Neumann et al., 2009). Within the current nosology, Pick’s disease is now used to describe specifically cases with microtubule-binding protein tau inclusions exhibiting round argyrophilic intracytoplasmic inclusions (Pick bodies) and ballooned achromatic neurons (Pick cells).

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

chronologically the material of memory which is related to their own experiences’ (p. 66). In 1957, the first systematic neuropathological classification of Pick’s disease was published (Lu¨ers and Spatz, 1957), which followed on the work by Gru¨nthal (1936). This classification reported four main centres of atrophy: orbital, temporal-polar, frontal convexity/frontal insula and Ammon’s horn. Detailed description of the pathological changes (gliosis, inclusion, demyelination) showed that some regions of the hippocampus (dentate, subiculum) were more affected than others (CA 1–4). Despite the pathological changes noted in the hippocampal region, these authors did not specifically report on the presence of memory disturbance during clinical examination. Delay and colleagues (1957a, b) contrasted the clinical presentations in 12 cases with Pick’s disease (seven with pathological confirmation) and 24 cases with Alzheimer’s disease (11 with pathological confirmation). The clinical presentation of Pick’s disease was described as ‘univocal, with a striking monotony’. Importantly, however, a disturbance of memory unrelated to attentional deficits was reported in 9 of their 12 cases with Pick’s disease, with five presenting with prominent memory deficits. Pathological investigations revealed bilateral frontotemporal atrophy localized frontally in three cases. In three other cases, neuronal loss was present in CA fields, accompanied by atrophy, neuronal loss and gliosis of the amygdala, as well as neuronal inclusions in the presubiculum in one case. In the early 1970s, a group in Geneva, Switzerland, conducted painstaking and important work that has received limited attention but is highly germane to the issues under discussion here (Constantinidis et al., 1974; Tissot et al., 1975). These authors presented the clinicopathological correlations of 32 cases of Pick’s disease, grouped according to the location and type of underlying pathology. In 18 cases, behavioural changes (‘moria, bipolar humour’) were reported together with anterograde memory disorder (‘amne´sie de fixation’), from early in the course of the disease. The memory deficit was described to be akin to that seen in Korsakoff’s and Alzheimer’s disease, and characterized by temporal and spatial disorientation, confabulations and poor recognition memory. In all these cases, atrophy was present in the middle temporal region, involving the hippocampus and other limbic structures, plus the anterior temporal lobe extending into the orbitofrontal regions. In over half of these cases, argentophilic neuronal inclusions (Pick bodies) and neuronal swelling (Pick cells) were observed in the subiculum, Sommer’s sector of the hippocampus, and dentate gyrus with gliosis and demyelination of fornix. In addition, significant correlations were reported between severity of pathology and memory performance. Importantly, the authors state that the failure to recognize the memory deficit in Pick’s disease was often masked by language reduction and behavioural changes. This presentation was contrasted with other cases where the atrophy and pathology was found predominantly in the prefrontal cortex and more particularly in the dorsolateral prefrontal region. In this group, clinical signs included memory inefficiency characterized clinically by preserved spatial orientation but impaired temporal orientation and inability to order events in the correct order (‘dyschronologic amnesia, frontal type’). This type of memory

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Episodic memory in FTD

Recent evidence This section reviews the current evidence on episodic memory in FTD. Unlike the historical studies reviewed above, most recent investigations have been group studies. We start by reviewing the evidence on retrograde memory (i.e. personal memories from the past) before examining studies investigating anterograde memory (i.e. acquisition of novel episodic memories). The distinction between retrograde and anterograde memory is deliberate as different patterns of performance for both types of memory have been reported in FTD subtypes.

Retrograde memory

Semantic dementia Early studies in semantic dementia reported significantly better recall for events from the recent than from the more distant past (Snowden et al., 1996; Graham and Hodges, 1997; Hodges and Graham, 1998; Graham et al., 1999; Nestor et al., 2002; Hou et al., 2005). This profile of preserved recent episodic memories is the reverse of the temporal gradient seen in amnesia and Alzheimer’s disease (Ribot, 1882; Greene et al., 1995) where memory for recent events is most vulnerable arguably because of the pathological changes in medial temporal lobe structures, notably the hippocampi and surrounding structures (Scoville and Milner, 1957). Detailed investigations have suggested that autobiographical memory in semantic dementia is characterized by a temporal ‘step-function’ whereby recall of memories for 18 months to 2 years prior to testing is maintained, but memories beyond this recent period show a flat, impaired retrieval function, most likely due to the progressive semanticization of the memory traces (Graham and Hodges, 1997; Hodges and Graham, 1998). The finding of a step-function is, however, not universal with some reports of a relative intact episodic memory across all life periods in semantic dementia (Westmacott et al., 2001; Moss et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2006; Maguire et al., 2010). One potential reason for these discrepant findings is that the integrity of remote memory in semantic dementia may have been underestimated in some studies because of these patients’ pervasive comprehension and language production difficulties. This position implies that such language problems will be overcome when patients are provided with specific cueing or probing of event

| 681

information compared with free recall. Evidence of autobiographical memory performance in semantic dementia matching that of healthy controls following the provision of additional cueing or probing corroborates this view (Moss et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2006). A recent study that used the same methodology as McKinnon and colleagues (2006), however, again found evidence for a ‘step-function’ and no additional benefit from cueing in a large group of patients with semantic dementia (n = 25) (Irish et al., 2011). It remains unclear whether these conflicting results may be due to different sample sizes [case studies (Moss et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2006) versus group study (Irish et al., 2011)] and associated statistical power, or reflect other variables, such as disease severity effects (Matuszewski et al., 2009; Maguire et al., 2010) (Fig. 1). The finding of preserved recent autobiographical memory certainly accords with clinical experience in that patients with semantic dementia appear much more aware of recent events in their life than those with Alzheimer’s disease (Hodges, 2001; Rosen et al., 2004). In contrast, when questioned about more distant life events, responses appear reasonable but are stereotyped and lack specific content. At a theoretical level, the temporal step-function reported by some studies has implications for the debate between competing theories of the neural mechanisms underlying long-term memory storage. Hodges and Graham (2001) argue that the reverse temporal step-function seen in semantic dementia lends support to the hypothesis that the hippocampus and related structures may play a time-limited role in encoding and storage of episodic memories. The greater impairment to recall distant, as opposed to recent, memories suggests that neocortical areas of the temporal lobes may be the critical location for our enduring stores of autobiographical and semantic memory, independent of the hippocampal complex (Graham and Hodges, 1997; Hodges and Graham, 1998; Graham, 1999). Proponents of the multiple-trace theory of memory storage (Nadel and Moscovitch, 1997; Moscovitch and Nadel, 1998; Winocur et al., 2010), which advocate that the hippocampal complex is involved in the storage of a memory for the whole of its lifetime, dispute Graham’s (1999) interpretation of the evidence arising from studies on semantic dementia. They argue that the impairment to distant memories may be attributable to dysfunctional strategic retrieval processes due to frontal cortical damage, which would affect distant memories more so than those from the recent past (Nadel and Moscovitch, 1997). It is not clear, however, how defective strategic retrieval processes would result in the reverse temporal ‘step-function’ documented in semantic dementia (Graham, 1999). This debate remains unresolved (Moscovitch and Nadel, 1999), although recent functional imaging findings (Maguire et al., 2010), which are discussed below, shed some new light on this issue. Both theories of episodic long-term memory agree that the hippocampus and related structures are heavily involved in the acquisition and, at least temporary, storage of novel episodic memories. Patients with semantic dementia typically show severe temporal lobe atrophy including the perirhinal cortex and temporal pole (Chan et al., 2001; Galton et al., 2001; Rosen et al., 2002a; Davies et al., 2004; Du et al., 2007; Mion et al., 2010).

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

A large proportion of studies investigating episodic memory in FTD have focused on memory for past events and experiences. In clinical practice, enquiries about personal events in the patient’s life are often unsystematic. To address this issue, a number of investigators have devised autobiographical memory tests (e.g. Kopelman et al., 1989; Levine et al., 2002). These tests typically comprise a free recall procedure, which allows participants to generate events across different life periods, followed, in some instances, by a cued recall procedure to probe further for contextual-rich event information. Despite these systematic approaches, conflicting findings have emerged in FTD subtypes, particularly in semantic dementia where most of the studies have been conducted. Findings from each FTD subtype are reviewed in the following sections.

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

682

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

performance across time periods according to the type of tasks. The step-function with better performance for recent events is present under free recall condition only (A) but not under probed recall (B) or high retrieval support condition (C). In C, the triangles denote performance of semantic dementia Patient A.A. tested on two separate occasions, 12 months apart; the squares denoted the performance of semantic dementia Patient B.B.; and the open circles the performance of healthy controls. A and B are from Irish et al. (2011), C is from McKinnon et al. (2006). Reprinted with permission from Elsevier. AD = Alzheimer’s disease; bvFTD = behavioural variant FTD; SD = semantic dementia.

Although earlier studies (e.g. Galton et al., 2001) suggested a selective sparing of the hippocampus, more recent objective measures have shown that the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex are substantially involved in the pathological process (Chan et al., 2001; Davies et al., 2004, 2005; Williams et al., 2005). Surprisingly, despite such pervasive hippocampal involvement, patients with semantic dementia show partial or complete

autobiographical memories in comparison to patients with Alzheimer’s disease presenting with a comparable degree of hippocampal atrophy. One plausible explanation for this finding is the differential atrophy severity within the hippocampus between the two patient groups: patients with semantic dementia show a rostral–caudal atrophy gradient in the hippocampus, with anterior parts most affected, while patients with Alzheimer’s disease show equivalent atrophy across all hippocampal parts (Chan et al., 2001; Davies et al., 2005). Another possible explanation is that patients with Alzheimer’s disease exhibit dysfunction in additional brain areas relevant for supporting autobiographical memory processing, such as posterior cingulate and precuneus cortices (Nestor et al., 2003). Importantly, these brain regions have been implicated in episodic memory disturbance in neurodegenerative diseases (Nestor et al., 2006) and shown to be consistently activated in autobiographical memory functional MRI studies in healthy volunteers (e.g. Maguire et al., 2001; Svoboda et al., 2006). A critical study by Nestor et al. (2006) compared structural and metabolic changes in key limbic structures across semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease groups. Although the degree of hippocampal atrophy on quantitative MRI and hypometabolism on FDG-PET was equivalent in the two groups, patients with semantic dementia showed sparing of the precuneus, which was severely hypometabolic in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. A recent case study further explored the neural correlates of autobiographical memory retrieval in semantic dementia using functional MRI (Maguire et al., 2010). In this patient, autobiographical memories were relatively intact initially but deteriorated over time with no evidence of a temporal gradient. The overall autobiographical performance remained relatively robust with increasing disease severity, which was corroborated by residual hippocampal and temporal lobe activation during recollection of autobiographical information. Interestingly, Maguire and colleagues (2010) also reported activation in the precuneus, which appears upregulated after the hippocampal activation was compromised by atrophy. Involvement of the precuneus confirms the importance of extra-hippocampal regions in autobiographical memory recall (Fig. 2). It is therefore not surprising that patients with Alzheimer’s disease, who show atrophy and dysfunction in both regions, will exhibit the greatest deficits in autobiographical memory from an early disease stage.

Progressive non-fluent aphasia To our knowledge, only one study systematically investigated autobiographical memory in PNFA (McKinnon et al., 2008), which may be due to the inherent language production deficits impacting on any verbal recall performance in these patients. A mild autobiographical memory impairment was reported, which was abolished by providing additional probing of event information. The authors concluded that the autobiographical memory deficits were secondary to pervasive expressive language difficulties in these patients. The relatively intact performance after probing further suggested that the memory retrieval processes and medial temporal lobe functions were not impaired in PNFA. This conclusion is corroborated by voxel-based morphometry studies which have demonstrated absence of medial temporal lobe

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Figure 1 Variability in the pattern of autobiographical memory

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Episodic memory in FTD

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

| 683

dementia 43 years. Top: Loss of activation in the hippocampal region over time. Bottom: Increased activation in posterior brain regions with disease progression. Autobiographical memory was relatively intact in this patient until late in the disease with no evidence of temporal gradient. Adapted from Maguire et al. (2010) with permission from Elsevier.

atrophy in this group (e.g. Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004; Rohrer et al., 2009).

Behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia Systematic investigations of episodic memory in behavioural variant FTD are sparse because the presence of amnesia remains a diagnostic exclusion criterion for the disease (Neary et al., 1998; Rascovsky et al., 2011). From a clinical perspective, however, growing evidence has shown that many patients with behavioural variant FTD present with complaints (either self- or carer-report) of memory problems. Around 10% of pathologically confirmed cases with behavioural variant FTD have marked episodic memory deficits in the initial stages of disease (Hodges et al., 2004), and occasional cases have severe amnesia (Caine et al., 2001; Graham et al., 2005). A clinicopathological study (Rosen et al., 2002b) confirmed that although the absence of severe amnesia markedly increased the odds for FTD, the converse was not true. Although patients with dementia with primary memory complaints are likely to have Alzheimer’s disease, the specificity of this complaint is low (Rascovsky et al., 2007, 2011). Investigations of retrograde autobiographical memory in behavioural variant FTD have also yielded inconsistent results. An early investigation using the Autobiographical Memory Interview failed to find impairment (Nestor et al., 2002) but recent studies that distinguished specific from generic information found uniform impairment across all time periods with a loss of specific details but no temporal gradient (Thomas-Anterion et al., 2000; Piolino et al., 2003, 2007; Matuszewski et al., 2006). Similarly, Irish and colleagues (2011) showed impaired recall of specific and contextualrich autobiographical memory across all time periods, regardless of how much cueing was provided. This pattern was interpreted as reflecting a disorder of strategic retrieval processes. Indeed, several studies have shown that autobiographical memory deficits in behavioural variant FTD are strongly correlated to executive dysfunction, which is a common neuropsychological feature of this group

(Piolino et al., 2003, 2007; Matuszewski et al., 2006). This relation is further corroborated by findings of dorsolateral and ventromedial atrophy on structural MRI (Seeley, 2008), which would explain the dysexecutive deficits in these patients. Additional evidence of dysfunction in these regions and their importance for autobiographical memory processing comes from metabolic neuroimaging. Recall of specific autobiographical memories was associated with resting-state FDG-PET activation in the orbitofrontal cortex in a group of patients with behavioural variant FTD (Piolino et al., 2007). In contrast, generic or semantic event information was related to anterior temporal lobe activation. These findings further substantiate the contribution of prefrontal cortex regions, and in particular orbitofrontal cortex, to autobiographical memory performance. Disturbance in these regions leads to a retrieval access deficit for all autobiographical memories regardless of their time of acquisition. In summary, retrograde memory performance differs across FTD subtypes. It is in semantic dementia that evidence is most variable and complex. While some studies have reported impairment for most recent retrograde memories with more remote memories being relatively intact, others show relative intact performance across all time periods tested in the early disease stages and an overall impairment later on in the disease. It is currently not clear which factors cause these discrepant findings. One potential explanation is that most of the studies used only single cases with semantic dementia or small samples, while the only large semantic dementia group study (Irish et al., 2011) shows a temporal gradient. The limited data suggest that patients with PNFA are mostly intact for this type of episodic memory. In contrast, patients with behavioural variant FTD show an overall impairment of retrograde memories regardless of the time of acquisition. Recent findings suggest that the retrograde memory deficits in patients with behavioural variant FTD are strongly related to their executive deficits. In turn, this finding supports the view that retrograde memory deficits in this group are due to a failure in strategic

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Figure 2 Longitudinal changes in the pattern of brain activation during autobiographical memory retrieval in a patient with semantic

684

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

retrieval processes, which are mainly mediated by prefrontal cortex, brain regions showing the most severe atrophy in behavioural variant FTD.

Anterograde memory Semantic dementia

atrophy. Such a pattern was indeed observed, whereby patients with semantic dementia with predominant left-sided atrophy performed normally on non-verbal memory tasks, whereas those with predominant right-sided atrophy performed poorly on unfamiliar face memory tasks, such as the Warrington Recognition Memory Test. Thus, examination of anterograde memory in semantic dementia needs to take into account the laterality of greatest brain atrophy, given its influence on test performance. As Warrington (1975) suggested, patients with semantic dementia appear to rely on perceptual information from the studied stimuli to assist their recognition memory. To follow-up on this observation, Graham and colleagues (2000) investigated recognition memory in patients with semantic dementia using colour pictures. Crucially, while some target items were perceptually identical at study and test (e.g. the same telephone), others were perceptually different (e.g. telephones of different colours or shapes). Despite their profound semantic memory problems, the patients with semantic dementia showed relatively preserved episodic memory for perceptually identical pictures. In contrast, recognition performance was much poorer for the items that were perceptually different at study and test, some patients performing at chance level. These findings demonstrate that patients with semantic dementia rely, therefore, heavily on perceptual inputs whereas in healthy people both semantic and perceptual features can be used to encode new information. Not surprisingly, patients with semantic dementia show high rates of false recognition when perceptually similar objects are used for episodic memory testing (Simons et al., 2005). Further experimentation demonstrated that most perceptual aspects (colour, viewpoint, exemplar types) are important for object recognition memory performance in semantic dementia, with the exception of size (Ikeda et al., 2006). Patients with semantic dementia made false recognitions in all conditions, except the size change condition. These findings also explain the impaired recognition memory for words found in semantic dementia, given the little distinctive perceptual features written words have in comparison to pictures. The perirhinal cortex, which is consistently affected in semantic dementia (Davies et al., 2004), emerges as the critical neuroanatomical region supporting episodic memory under perceptually difficult conditions. The role of perirhinal cortex in memory has been known for quite some time, with experimentally induced lesions of this region leading to severe recognition memory deficits in monkeys (e.g. Buckley and Gaffan, 1997). Recent evidence, however, suggests that such perirhinal cortex damage may cause a higher perceptual deficit, rather than a memory deficit per se (Bussey and Saksida, 2007). The perirhinal cortex appears critically involved in analysing and representing complex conjunctions of the features that comprise an individual object, which in turn affects the recognition of an object in a memory test. This position could explain the change in memory performance following perceptual manipulations (Graham et al., 2000; Simons et al., 2001). Indeed, perceptual tasks with virtually no memory load that use concurrent visual discrimination or object oddity judgements have been shown to be severely impaired in semantic dementia (Barense et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2005a, b, but see Levy et al., 2005; Shrager et al., 2006). These findings provide further evidence that visual memory problems in semantic dementia may be related

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

The first systematic neuropsychological assessment of anterograde episodic memory in FTD involved three patients with semantic dementia who demonstrated impaired recall of the Wechsler Memory Scale short story and a virtually complete failure in free recall of 10-word lists (Warrington, 1975). In addition, these patients with semantic dementia performed as poorly as four amnesic patients on word and face stimuli on forced-choice recognition memory tests. When recognition memory for paintings was tested, however, all patients with semantic dementia scored in the normal range, in contrast to the amnesic patients. Warrington speculated that the patients with semantic dementia might have utilized perceptual features, such as colours, to remember the paintings, whereas memory for words relies essentially on semantic information. As Warrington indicated: ‘the patients reported here are by no means as amnesic as a patient with a severe amnesic syndrome, although their verbal memory is far from normal’ (Warrington, 1975, p. 653). This classic description still best summarizes the distinction between verbal and visual anterograde memory performance in semantic dementia, in particular for recall-based tests. Verbal recall tests have the inherent confound of requiring spoken output from the participant that is affected by the semantic knowledge loss and pervasive anomia in semantic dementia. Not surprisingly, performance on word-list learning, such as the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, or recall of a story (e.g. Wechsler short story passages) can be particularly impaired in semantic dementia and can make a distinction from Alzheimer’s disease difficult (e.g. Perry and Hodges, 2000). Patients perform in general better on tests using non-verbal material, such as the Rey complex figure test, and show overall better performance on recognition tests. From the late 1990s, a series of studies investigated systematically aspects of anterograde memory in semantic dementia. One of the first studies used black and white line drawings of real and fictitious animals (Graham et al., 1997). While no significant difference between a group of patients with semantic dementia and healthy controls was found on a test of recognition memory for these drawings, patients with semantic dementia were significantly impaired when asked to indicate during the study task whether the animals were real or not. This observation was confirmed using colour pictures of objects (Graham et al., 2000) and photographs of people (Simons et al., 2001). Scahill and colleagues (2005) went on to address the issue of laterality of atrophy and episodic memory processing in semantic dementia. Anatomical distinctions in semantic dementia are important in that predominant leftversus right-sided patients with semantic dementia can present with varying degrees of anomia. Patients with predominant left-sided atrophy are assumed to perform worse on verbal tasks than non-verbal tasks, whereas the opposite should be true for patients with semantic dementia with predominant right-sided

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Episodic memory in FTD

Progressive non-fluent aphasia To our knowledge, only one study has examined performance of patients with PNFA on neuropsychological memory tests (Libon et al., 2007). This study showed that patients with PNFA performed significantly better than patients with Alzheimer’s disease on the Verbal Serial List Learning Test. In contrast, patients with PNFA experienced working memory deficits, which are dependent on prefrontal cortex integrity. Clearly, future investigations of PNFA memory performance are needed, in particular in light of recent findings indicating that a subset of patients with PNFA have underlying Alzheimer pathology (Hu et al., 2010).

Behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia Anterograde episodic memory function in behavioural variant FTD has been investigated systematically only recently. As mentioned before, the scarcity of such studies arises from the fact that amnesia is an exclusion criterion for behavioural variant FTD (Neary et al., 1998). While patients with behavioural variant FTD are often impaired on memory tasks compared with healthy controls (e.g. Pachana et al. 1996; Giovagnoli et al. 2008), these deficits are variable and tend to be overshadowed by the predominant behavioural changes. Compared with Alzheimer’s disease, patients with behavioural variant FTD are generally reported to perform better on verbal and visuospatial anterograde memory tasks (Elfgren et al., 1994; Frisoni et al., 1995; Pachana et al., 1996;

| 685

Lindau et al., 1998; Thomas-Anterion et al., 2000; Rascovsky et al., 2002; Kramer et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2003; Rosen et al., 2004; Perri et al., 2005; Heidler-Gary et al., 2007; Libon et al., 2007; Giovagnoli et al., 2008) and do not normally show the accelerated forgetting under delayed free recall conditions typically found in Alzheimer’s disease (Hodges et al., 1999; Perry and Hodges, 2000; Glosser et al., 2002; Wicklund et al., 2006; Hutchinson and Mathias, 2007). Importantly, however, exceptions exist and patients with behavioural variant FTD with neuropathological confirmation at post-mortem have presented with severe amnesia in some cases (Caine et al., 2001; Graham et al., 2005). Relatively intact memory performance, at least in the early stages of behavioural variant FTD, has also been reported on complex associative memory tasks, such as the computerized Paired Associate Learning Test from the CANTAB (Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery). This task, in which subjects are required to learn the association between coloured patterns and spatial locations, is very sensitive to early Alzheimer’s disease (Swainson et al., 2001; Blackwell et al., 2004), whereas patients with early behavioural variant FTD tend to perform within the normal range (Lee et al., 2003). A similar dissociation between Alzheimer’s disease and behavioural variant FTD has been reported on a face-location association task (Clague et al., 2005). In contrast, no reliable difference between Alzheimer’s disease and behavioural variant FTD has been found on forced-choice recognition and free recall for verbal and non-verbal material (Glosser et al., 2002). A selective retrieval disorder, potentially caused by the known attention problems, was postulated for the impaired memory in behavioural variant FTD (Glosser et al., 2002). In addition, a dissociation has been reported in patients with behavioural variant FTD with a relatively preserved recognition memory but impaired temporal source memory (remembering whether an item was shown in list A or list B) (Simons et al., 2002), in contrast to patients with Alzheimer’s disease who are typically impaired on both recognition and source memory tasks (Multhaup and Balota, 1997). Source memory performance is generally attributed to prefrontal cortex function (e.g. Duarte et al., 2011). Whether the variable memory performance observed in behavioural variant FTD is due to the prefrontal cortex atrophy or, like Alzheimer’s disease, due to hippocampal atrophy is unknown. Evidence regarding the severity of hippocampal atrophy in behavioural variant FTD is mixed. At post-mortem, significant hippocampal atrophy has been reported in patients with FTD, even early in the course of the disease (van Mansvelt, 1954; Broe et al., 2003; Kril and Halliday, 2004) (Fig. 3). Medial temporal atrophy has also been found on neuroimaging (Seeley, 2008; Seeley et al., 2008; Whitwell et al., 2009), although no hypoperfusion has been reported in this region in behavioural variant FTD (Kanda et al., 2008). Involvement of medial temporal regions in behavioural variant FTD remains somewhat contentious but a consensus exists on the early involvement of mesial and orbital prefrontal cortical regions (Rosen et al., 2002a; Seeley, 2008). Questions, however, remain regarding the variability in the pattern of change in behavioural variant FTD and whether the anterior temporal and/or parietal atrophy, which is found in addition to the frontal atrophy, is dependent on the underlying pathology (Whitwell et al., 2009, 2011). Whether

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

to perception rather than to memory per se. This finding could further explain the difficulty patients with semantic dementia experience with face recognition, as a more thorough ‘feature conjunction analysis’ is necessary to discriminate among similar faces successfully. Importantly, however, anterograde memory deficits in semantic dementia are unlikely to be due solely to higher perceptual deficits caused by perirhinal damage but rather to a combined perirhinal and hippocampal atrophy. Performance on memory tests that are highly sensitive to hippocampal damage, such as associative memory or source memory, is impaired in semantic dementia (Simons et al., 2002; Clague et al., 2005). Tests of high perceptual/perirhinal functioning may potentially be helpful in discriminating semantic dementia from Alzheimer’s disease, as this function is relatively intact in Alzheimer’s disease, while both patient groups are impaired on hippocampal-dependent tests. Another promising avenue to discriminate patients with semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease is the use of ‘naturalistic’ episodic memory tasks with incidental encoding of information. An elegant study demonstrated that patients with semantic dementia remembered most spatial (e.g. ‘Did I sit in this chair?’), temporal (e.g. ‘How long did I stay?’) and event (e.g. ‘Did I wear a brooch yesterday?’) details from the examiner’s visit to their home, even after a 24-h delay in contrast to patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Adlam et al., 2009). Similarly, topographical memory was found to be relatively intact in semantic dementia but very impaired in Alzheimer’s disease using a virtual reality task (Pengas et al., 2010). Importantly, in this study, performance on a virtual route learning task in combination with naming performance resulted in a complete discrimination of patients with semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease.

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

686

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

logical confirmation of frontotemporal lobar degeneration pathology. Significant atrophy is present involving the hippocampus and the middle temporal region on coronal MRI (A). At post-mortem, significant neuronal loss was found in the CA1 field of the hippocampus (B) and ubiquitin-positive staining (C). Adapted from Graham et al. (2005) with permission from Oxford University Press.

this pathological heterogeneity is also reflected in the memory performance of these patients remains unclear. Existing evidence would suggest that cases with severe memory disturbance at presentation appear to have pathological changes associated with

TDP-43 protein deposition (Graham et al., 2005) (Fig. 3). A critical issue in the next few years will be the development of reliable biomarkers capable of detecting those with underlying tau versus TDP-43 or FUS pathology, recent studies showing promising results (Piguet et al., 2011; Schofield et al., 2011). It may then be possible to relate the clinical phenotype, including the degree and nature of episodic memory impairment to the pathological type in vivo. The reported dissociation between intact implicit but impaired explicit anterograde memory in behavioural variant FTD (Pasquier et al., 2001) provides further support for a disturbance of prefrontal mediated memory function in these patients. Critically, this study demonstrated that retrieval cues provided greater benefit in the explicit memory condition for patients with behavioural variant FTD than for Alzheimer’s disease. This finding prompted the suggestion that memory dysfunction in behavioural variant FTD resulted from frontally dependent, defective cognitive (retrieval) control processes rather than true amnesia (e.g. Collette et al., 2010). Further support arises from neuroimaging investigations showing that disturbance in the strategic aspects of episodic memory recall and retrieval processes are associated with prefrontal cortex atrophy, in particular the orbitofrontal cortex (Kramer et al., 2005; Pennington et al., 2011) as well as reduced metabolism in prefrontal, parietal and posterior cingulate cortices in patients with behavioural variant FTD (Bastin et al., 2011). The failure of these prefrontally mediated retrieval control mechanisms also explains the increased false recognition rates (de Boysson et al., 2011) and confabulations found in behavioural variant FTD (Nedjam et al., 2004). The inconsistent findings across studies regarding episodic memory in behavioural variant FTD are also likely to be due to the inclusion of patients who do not have a progressive neurodegenerative disease. Recent studies have identified a subset of patients who meet current clinical criteria for behavioural variant FTD, yet show little or no progression over a decade or more (Davies et al., 2006; Kipps et al., 2007b, 2010; Piguet et al., 2009). These patients with non-progressor or ‘phenocopy’ behavioural variant FTD mimic the clinical features of behavioural variant FTD but lack the typical brain atrophy found on MRI and demonstrate preserved glucose metabolism on PET (Davies et al., 2006; Kipps et al., 2007a, 2009) and the pathology of these cases remains unclear. The prevalence of these phenocopy cases in existing studies is unknown but their inclusion is likely to underplay the magnitude of the deficits in progressive, or patients with ‘real’, behavioural variant FTD. This position was confirmed in a recent study in which patients with behavioural variant FTD, who had been followed over many years, were grouped according to their disease progression status (real versus phenocopy behavioural variant FTD) and their anterograde episodic memory performance at presentation compared with Alzheimer’s disease (Hornberger et al., 2010a). Patients with progressive behavioural variant FTD were nearly as impaired as patients with Alzheimer’s disease on most verbal and visual neuropsychological memory measures, in contrast to the phenocopy cases who showed only mild deficits compared with control participants (Fig. 4). The same study also showed that patients with Alzheimer’s disease scored worse on delayed recall tests and

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Figure 3 Patient presenting with severe amnesia and patho-

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Episodic memory in FTD

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

| 687

on orientation scores of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-Revised compared with the patients with behavioural variant FTD. Importantly, these findings were replicated in an independent prospective sample (Pennington et al., 2011) (Fig. 4), which further showed that prefrontal atrophy ratings in patients with real behavioural variant FTD correlated with the memory performance. Interestingly, patients with behavioural variant FTD and Alzheimer’s disease showed hippocampal atrophy of similar severity. The pattern of atrophy, however, differed between the two groups with behavioural variant FTD showing more pronounced anterior than posterior hippocampal atrophy compared with Alzheimer’s disease (see also Laakso et al. 2000). Disturbance in both prefrontal and medial temporal lobe regions are therefore likely to contribute to the memory impairments seen in patients with behavioural variant FTD, with the relative contribution varying across patients, possibly related to the site of underlying pathology or to the disease severity.

Memory tests that are capable of differentiating between these anatomical contributions are required. In summary, studies investigating anterograde memory in behavioural variant FTD report inconsistent results. Possible reasons for the discrepant findings are (i) the use of memory tests which place different demands on prefrontal versus medial temporal lobe functioning, such as recall versus recognition; and (ii) the potential admixture of real patients with behavioural variant FTD with phenocopy cases, resulting in some studies to an underrepresentation of anterograde memory deficits in this patient group. Taking all the evidence reviewed into account indicates that patients with true behavioural variant FTD can have memory deficits of the same magnitude as seen in Alzheimer’s disease. This finding poses a challenge to the widely used diagnostic criteria for behavioural variant FTD (Neary et al., 1998), which states severe memory deficits as an exclusion criterion, as well as for the recently revised criteria (Rascovsky et al., 2011)

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Figure 4 Pattern of performance on different tests of episodic memory in patients with behavioural variant FTD (bvFTD) compared with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), phenocopy cases and controls. Patients with real behavioural variant FTD are impaired to a similar degree to patients with Alzheimer’s disease. In contrast, performance of phenocopy cases is similar to that of controls. (A) Overall memory performance, (B) performance across groups on various memory tasks. A is reprinted from Hornberger et al. (2010a) with permission from Wolters Kluwer Health. B is reprinted from Pennington et al. (2011) with permission from IOS Press. AD = patients with Alzheimer’s disease; PR = patients with real behavioural variant FTD; PH = phenocopy cases; Cntrl = healthy controls.

688

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

which advocate a relative sparing of episodic memory on neuropsychological testing.

Conclusions and future directions

and Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis (Hornberger et al., 2010a). In contrast, recognition memory scores appear the least predictive markers of episodic memory differences between the groups. Improved clinical diagnostic accuracy may also be achieved using executive function tests, such as the Hayling test of inhibition, which discriminate behavioural variant FTD from Alzheimer’s disease and from phenocopy cases to a high degree even in the early stages of the disease (Hornberger et al., 2008, 2010b). As indicated above, accurate assessment of memory performance has crucial implications for the clinical diagnosis of patients with FTD. The importance of evaluating memory performance within the context of other cognitive abilities, however, cannot be overemphasized, as presence/absence of deficits in other cognitive domains will inform as to the relative severity of the memory impairment and help reach a clinical diagnosis. For example, comparing episodic memory and temporal orientation integrity appear useful, as disorientation is commonly observed in Alzheimer’s disease but rarely so in behavioural variant FTD. Complementing clinical tests with imaging investigations, imaging neural correlates, in particular of prefrontal cortex regions, such as in Pennington and colleagues (2011) appear very promising in dissociating patients with behavioural variant FTD and Alzheimer’s disease memory deficits. Investigations of memory in FTD have improved our theoretical understanding of episodic memory processes. In particular, the retrograde memory findings in semantic dementia have informed and challenged memory consolidation theories. Further, results of anterograde memory investigations have addressed the interactions between memory (episodic and semantic) and high-level perceptual processes. The disentangling of these processes continues to advance our understanding of medial temporal lobe memory systems, which are critical to episodic memory functions. Similarly, studies examining anterograde memory in behavioural variant FTD have uncovered prefrontal cortex and/or medial temporal lobe contributions to memory deficits in this group. These contributions, however, remain difficult to tease apart with standard neuropsychological tests. Memory tests, such as within- and between-domain associative memory tasks, which are based on current experimental findings and can be easily applied in the clinics are needed. Such tests are capable of delineating between prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe, as well as among regions within the medial temporal lobe, specific memory processes. These new theoretical developments will inform clinical practice which will lead to improved diagnosis and improved patient management.

Acknowledgements We thank Prof. J. Hodges for helpful comments on earlier versions of the article.

Funding The Australian Research Council (ARC) Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders. ARC Research Fellowship

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

The evidence reviewed in this article demonstrates the relevance of episodic memory investigations in FTD with each subtype showing specific changes in retrograde and anterograde memory. In semantic dementia, the profile is one of significant retrograde memory deficits (with or without step-gradient) modulated by disease severity and of anterograde memory deficits that are affected by visuoperceptual aspects and the modality of the information to be remembered. In contrast, both retrograde and anterograde memory functions are mostly intact in PNFA, with the exception of a mild autobiographical memory deficit, which is abolished following the provision of retrieval cues. Finally, retrograde and anterograde memory performance is affected in behavioural variant FTD, and appears to be particularly sensitive to the co-occurrence of executive dysfunction. Importantly, aspects of this review are relevant for clinical practice. Patients with semantic dementia and behavioural variant FTD experience episodic memory problems that have been under recognized so far, likely due to their pervasive language and behavioural deficits. The historical and current evidence shows that some patients with FTD can have episodic memory problems similar to Alzheimer’s disease, which can make a diagnostic distinction difficult, particularly early in the disease course. More specifically, patients with semantic dementia exhibit retrograde and anterograde memory deficits, the severity of which varies according to the tests used. For clinicians, impaired performance on verbal but not visuospatial memory tests combined with a loss of semantic knowledge should raise the strong possibility of semantic dementia. Other features supportive of this diagnosis include preservation of visual memory particularly under forced-choice recognition format and preserved memory for temporal information and for recent autobiographical events. With regards to behavioural variant FTD, retrograde memory data demonstrate that behavioural variant FTD and Alzheimer’s disease show different autobiographical memory profiles, although such evidence remains difficult to gather in the clinic. More concerning, however, is the evidence that patients with behavioural variant FTD and Alzheimer’s disease can present with similar anterograde memory deficits on neuropsychological testing. This overlap becomes particularly relevant for the patients with Alzheimer’s disease who show additional executive dysfunction (e.g. Gleichgerrcht et al., 2010). In addition, the likely inclusion of phenocopy cases in previous studies will have underestimated the severity of the episodic memory impairment in this patient group, and cast further doubt on the validity of amnesia as a diagnostic exclusion criterion for the disease (although this point has been rectified somewhat in the recent revision of the clinical diagnostic criteria for behavioural variant FTD). Nevertheless, delayed recall memory tests, in particular of verbal material (e.g. word lists), emerge as a good predictor of behavioural variant FTD

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Episodic memory in FTD (DP110104202 to M.H.); National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia Clinical Career Development Award Fellowship (#510184 to O.P.).

References

| 689

Delay J, Brion S, Escourolle R. L’opposition anatomo-clinique des maladies de Pick et d’Alzheimer: Etude de 38 cas [Anatomo-clinical contrasts between Pick’s and Alzheimer’s diseases: 38 single-case studies]. La Presse Me´dicale 1957a; 65: 1495–7. Delay J, Brion S, Escourolle R. L’opposition anatomo-clinique des maladies de Pick et d’Alzheimer: Valeur diagnostique des examens comple´mentaires [Anatomo-clinical contrasts between Pick’s and Alzheimer’s diseases: Diagnostic use of complementary investigations]. La Presse Me´dicale 1957b; 65: 1515–8. Du AT, Schuff N, Kramer JH, Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Rankin K, et al. Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Brain 2007; 130: 1159–66. Duarte A, Henson RN, Graham KS. Stimulus content and the neural correlates of source memory. Brain Res 2011; 1373: 110–23. Elfgren C, Brun A, Gustafson L, Johanson A, Minthon L, Passant U, et al. Neuropsychological tests as discriminators between dementia of Alzheimer type and frontotemporal dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1994; 9: 635–42. Frisoni GB, Pizzolato G, Geroldi C, Rossato A, Bianchetti A, Trabucchi M. Dementia of the frontal type: neuropsychological and [99Tc]-HM-PAO SPET features. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1995; 8: 42–8. Galton CJ, Patterson K, Graham K, Lambon-Ralph MA, Williams G, Antoun N, et al. Differing patterns of temporal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neurology 2001; 57: 216–25. Giovagnoli AR, Erbetta A, Reati F, Bugiani O. Differential neuropsychological patterns of frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in a study of diagnostic concordance. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46: 1495–504. Gleichgerrcht E, Torralva T, Martinez D, Roca M, Manes F. Impact of executive dysfunction on verbal memory performance in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2011; 23: 79–85. Glosser G, Gallo JL, Clark CM, Grossman M. Memory encoding and retrieval in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 2002; 16: 190–6. Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, Ogar JM, Phengrasamy L, Rosen HJ, et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 2004; 55: 335–46. Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, Cappa SF, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 2011; 76: 1006–14. Graham KS. Semantic dementia: a challenge to the multiple-trace theory? Trends Cogn Sci 1999; 3: 85–7. Graham KS, Becker JT, Hodges JR. On the relationship between knowledge and memory for pictures: evidence from the study of patients with semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1997; 3: 534–44. Graham A, Davies R, Xuereb J, Halliday G, Kril J, Creasey H, et al. Pathologically proven frontotemporal dementia presenting with severe amnesia. Brain 2005; 128: 597–605. Graham KS, Hodges JR. Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex in long-term memory storage: evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 1997; 11: 77–89. Graham KS, Murre JM, Hodges JR. Episodic memory in semantic dementia: a computational approach based on the TraceLink model. Prog Brain Res 1999; 121: 47–65. Graham KS, Simons JS, Pratt KH, Patterson K, Hodges JR. Insights from semantic dementia on the relationship between episodic and semantic memory. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38: 313–24. Greene JD, Hodges JR, Baddeley AD. Autobiographical memory and executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33: 1647–70. Gru¨nthal E. Die pra¨senilen und senilen Erkrankungen des Gehirnes und des Ru¨ckenmarkes [The presenile and senile diseases of the brain and spinal cord]. In: Bumke O, Foerster O, editors. Handbuch der Neurologie. Berlin: Julius Springer; 1936. p. 446–509.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Adlam AL, Patterson K, Hodges JR. “I remember it as if it were yesterday”: memory for recent events in patients with semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47: 1344–51. Barense MD, Bussey TJ, Lee AC, Rogers TT, Davies RR, Saksida LM, et al. Functional specialization in the human medial temporal lobe. J Neurosci 2005; 25: 10239–46. Bastin C, Feyers D, Souchay C, Guillaume B, Pepin JL, Lemaire C, et al. Frontal and posterior cingulate metabolic impairment in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia with impaired autonoetic consciousness. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; doi: 10.1002/hbm/21282. Blackwell AD, Sahakian BJ, Vesey R, Semple JM, Robbins TW, Hodges JR. Detecting dementia: novel neuropsychological markers of pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease. Dem Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004; 17: 42–8. Broe M, Hodges JR, Schofield E, Shepherd CE, Kril JJ, Halliday GM. Staging disease severity in pathologically confirmed cases of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2003; 60: 1005–11. Brun A. Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. I. Neuropathology. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1987; 6: 193–208. Brun A, Englund E, Gustafson L, Passant U, Mann DMA, Neary D, et al. Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. The Lund and Manchester Groups. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994; 57: 416–8. Buckley MJ, Gaffan D. Impairment of visual object-discrimination learning after perirhinal cortex ablation. Behav Neurosci 1997; 111: 467–75. Bussey TJ, Saksida LM. Memory, perception, and the ventral visualperirhinal-hippocampal stream: thinking outside of the boxes. Hippocampus 2007; 17: 898–908. Caine D, Patterson K, Hodges JR, Heard R, Halliday G. Severe anterograde amnesia with extensive hippocampal degeneration in a case of rapidly progressive frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase 2001; 7: 57–64. Caron M. Etude clinique de la maladie de Pick [Clinical study of Pick’s disease]. Paris: Vigot Fre`res; 1934. Chan D, Fox NC, Scahill RI, Crum WR, Whitwell JL, Leschziner G, et al. Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 2001; 49: 433–42. Clague F, Dudas RB, Thompson SA, Graham KS, Hodges JR. Multidimensional measures of person knowledge and spatial associative learning: can these be applied to the differentiation of Alzheimer’s disease from frontotemporal and vascular dementia? Neuropsychologia 2005; 43: 1338–50. Collette F, van der Linden M, Salmon E. Dissociation between controlled and automatic processes in the behavioral variant of fronto-temporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2010; 22: 897–907. Constantinidis J, Richard J, Tissot R. Pick’s disease. Histological and clinical correlations. Eur Neurol 1974; 11: 208–17. Davies RR, Graham KS, Xuereb JH, Williams GB, Hodges JR. The human perirhinal cortex and semantic memory. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 20: 2441–6. Davies RR, Hodges JR, Kril JJ, Patterson K, Halliday GM, Xuereb JH. The pathological basis of semantic dementia. Brain 2005; 128: 1984–95. Davies RR, Kipps CM, Mitchell J, Kril JJ, Halliday GM, Hodges JR. Progression in frontotemporal dementia: identifying a benign behavioral variant by magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 2006; 63: 1627–31. de Boysson C, Belleville S, Phillips NA, Johns EK, Goupil D, Souchay C, et al. False recognition in Lewy-body disease and frontotemporal dementia. Brain Cogn 2011; 75: 111–8.

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

690

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

Kipps CM, Nestor PJ, Fryer TD, Hodges JR. Behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia: not all it seems? Neurocase 2007b; 13: 237–47. Kopelman MD, Wilson BA, Baddeley AD. The autobiographical memory interview: a new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in amnesic patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1989; 11: 724–44. Kramer JH, Jurik J, Sha SJ, Rankin KP, Rosen HJ, Johnson JK, et al. Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol 2003; 16: 211–8. Kramer JH, Rosen HJ, Du AT, Schuff N, Hollnagel C, Weiner MW, et al. Dissociations in hippocampal and frontal contributions to episodic memory performance. Neuropsychology 2005; 19: 799–805. Kril JJ, Halliday GM. Clinicopathological staging of frontotemporal dementia severity: correlation with regional atrophy. Dem Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004; 17: 311–5. Laakso MP, Frisoni GB, Ko¨no¨nen M, Mikkonen M, Beltramello A, Geroldi C, et al. Hippocampus and entorhinal cortex in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a morphometric MRI study. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47: 1056–63. Lee AC, Barense MD, Graham KS. The contribution of the human medial temporal lobe to perception: bridging the gap between animal and human studies. Q J Exp Psychol B 2005a; 58: 300–25. Lee AC, Buckley MJ, Pegman SJ, Spiers H, Scahill VL, Gaffan D, et al. Specialization in the medial temporal lobe for processing of objects and scenes. Hippocampus 2005b; 15: 782–97. Lee AC, Rahman S, Hodges JR, Sahakian BJ, Graham KS. Associative and recognition memory for novel objects in dementia: implications for diagnosis. Eur J Neurosci 2003; 18: 1660–70. Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol Aging 2002; 17: 677–89. Levy DA, Shrager Y, Squire LR. Intact visual discrimination of complex and feature-ambiguous stimuli in the absence of perirhinal cortex. Learn Mem 2005; 12: 61–6. Libon DJ, Xie SX, Moore P, Farmer J, Antani S, McCawley G, et al. Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2007; 68: 369–75. Lindau M, Almkvist O, Johansson SE, Wahlund LO. Cognitive and behavioral differentiation of frontal lobe degeneration of the non-Alzheimer type and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1998; 9: 205–13. Lo¨wenberg K. Pick’s disease: a clinicopathologic contribution. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1936; 36: 768–89. Lu¨ers T, Spatz H. Picksche Krankheit (progressive umschriebene Grosshirnatrophie) [Pick’s disease (progressive circumscribed brain atrophy)]. In: Scholz W, editor. Nervensystem. Berlin: Springer; 1957. p. 614–715. Mackenzie IR, Neumann M, Bigio EH, Cairns NJ, Alafuzoff I, Kril J, et al. Nomenclature and nosology for neuropathologic subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration: an update. Acta Neuropathol 2010; 119: 1–4. Maguire EA, Henson RN, Mummery CJ, Frith CD. Activity in prefrontal cortex, not hippocampus, varies parametrically with the increasing remoteness of memories. Neuroreport 2001; 12: 441–4. Maguire EA, Kumaran D, Hassabis D, Kopelman MD. Autobiographical memory in semantic dementia: a longitudinal fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48: 123–36. Matuszewski V, Piolino P, Belliard S, de la Sayette V, Laisney M, Lalevee C, et al. Patterns of autobiographical memory impairment according to disease severity in semantic dementia. Cortex 2009; 45: 456–72. Matuszewski V, Piolino P, de la Sayette V, Lalevee C, Pelerin A, Dupuy B, et al. Retrieval mechanisms for autobiographical memories: insights from the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 2386–97. McKinnon MC, Black SE, Miller B, Moscovitch M, Levine B. Autobiographical memory in semantic dementia: implication for

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Heidler-Gary J, Gottesman R, Newhart M, Chang S, Ken L, Hillis AE. Utility of behavioral versus cognitive measures in differentiating between subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007; 23: 184–93. Hodges JR. Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease): clinical features and assessment. Neurology 2001; 56: S6–10. Hodges JR, Davies R, Xuereb J, Casey B, Broe M, Bak T, et al. Clinicopathological correlates in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 2004; 56: 399–406. Hodges JR, Graham KS. A reversal of the temporal gradient for famous person knowledge in semantic dementia: implications for the neural organisation of long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 803–25. Hodges JR, Graham KS. Episodic memory: insights from semantic dementia. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2001; 356: 1423–34. Hodges JR, Miller B. The classification, genetics and neuropathology of frontotemporal dementia. Introduction to the special topic papers: part I. Neurocase 2001; 7: 31–5. Hodges JR, Patterson K, Ward R, Garrard P, Bak T, Perry R, et al. The differentiation of semantic dementia and frontal lobe dementia (temporal and frontal variants of frontotemporal dementia) from early Alzheimer’s disease: a comparative neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology 1999; 13: 31–40. Hornberger M, Piguet O, Graham AJ, Nestor PJ, Hodges JR. How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia? Neurology 2010a; 74: 472–9. Hornberger M, Piguet O, Kipps C, Hodges JR. Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2008; 71: 1481–8. Hornberger M, Savage S, Hsieh S, Mioshi E, Piguet O, Hodges JR. Orbitofrontal dysfunction discriminates behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease. Dem Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010b; 30: 547–52. Hou CE, Miller BL, Kramer JH. Patterns of autobiographical memory loss in dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005; 20: 809–15. Hu WT, McMillan C, Libon D, Leight S, Forman M, Lee VM, et al. Multimodal predictors for Alzheimer disease in nonfluent primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 2010; 75: 595–602. Hutchinson AD, Mathias JL. Neuropsychological deficits in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analytic review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007; 78: 917–28. Ikeda M, Patterson K, Graham KS, Ralph MA, Hodges JR. A horse of a different colour: do patients with semantic dementia recognise different versions of the same object as the same? Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 566–75. Irish M, Hornberger M, Lah S, Miller L, Pengas G, Nestor PJ, et al. Profiles of recent autobiographical memory retrieval in semantic dementia, behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 2694–702. Kahn E, Thompson LJ. Concerning Pick’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 1934; 13: 937–46. Kanda T, Ishii K, Uemura T, Miyamoto N, Yoshikawa T, Kono AK, et al. Comparison of grey matter and metabolic reductions in frontotemporal dementia using FDG-PET and voxel-based morphometric MR studies. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2008; 35: 2227–34. Kipps CM, Davies RR, Mitchell J, Kril JJ, Halliday GM, Hodges JR. Clinical significance of lobar atrophy in frontotemporal dementia: application of an MRI visual rating scale. Dem Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007a; 23: 334–42. Kipps CM, Hodges JR, Hornberger M. Nonprogressive behavioural frontotemporal dementia: recent developments and clinical implications of the ‘bvFTD phenocopy syndrome’. Curr Opin Neurol 2010; 23: 628–32. Kipps CM, Nestor PJ, Acosta-Cabronero J, Arnold R, Hodges JR. Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: the role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain 2009; 132: 592–603.

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Episodic memory in FTD

| 691

Pick A. Senile Hirnatrophie als Grundlage von Herderscheinungen. Wiener klinische Wochenschrift 1901; 14: 16–7. Translated by: Girling DM, Markova IS. Senile cerebral atrophy as the origin of focal symptoms. Hist Psychiatry 1995; 6: 533–37. Pick A. Zur Symptomatologie der linksseitigen Schla¨fenappenatrophie. Monatschrift fu¨r Psychiatrie und Neurologie 1904; 16: 377–88. Translated by Girling DM, Berrios GE. On the symptomatology of left-sided temporal lobe atrophy. Hist Psychiatry 1997; 8: 149–59. Piguet O, Hornberger M, Shelley BP, Kipps CM, Hodges JR. Sensitivity of current criteria for the diagnosis of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2009; 72: 732–7. Piguet O, Petersen A, Yin Ka Lam B, Gabery S, Murphy K, Hodges JR, et al. Eating and hypothalamus changes in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 2011; 69: 312–9. Piolino P, Chetelat G, Matuszewski V, Landeau B, Mezenge F, Viader F, et al. In search of autobiographical memories: a PET study in the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45: 2730–43. Piolino P, Desgranges B, Belliard S, Matuszewski V, Lalevee C, De la Sayette V, et al. Autobiographical memory and autonoetic consciousness: triple dissociation in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain 2003; 126: 2203–19. Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Kipps CM, Johnson JK, Seeley WW, Mendez MF, et al. Diagnostic criteria for the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD): current limitations and future directions. Alzh Dis Assoc Disord 2007; 21: S14–8. Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, Mendez MF, Kramer JH, Neuhaus J, et al. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 2011; 134: 2456–77. Rascovsky K, Salmon DP, Ho GJ, Galasko D, Peavy GM, Hansen LA, et al. Cognitive profiles differ in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and AD. Neurology 2002; 58: 1801–8. Ribot T. Disorders of memory. New York: Appleton; 1882. Rohrer JD, Warren JD, Modat M, Ridgway GR, Douiri A, Rossor MN, et al. Patterns of cortical thinning in the language variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2009; 72: 1562–9. Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Goldman WP, Perry RJ, Schuff N, Weiner M, et al. Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Neurology 2002a; 58: 198–208. Rosen HJ, Hartikainen KM, Jagust W, Kramer JH, Reed BR, Cummings JL, et al. Utility of clinical criteria in differentiating frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) from AD. Neurology 2002b; 58: 1608–15. Rosen HJ, Narvaez JM, Hallam B, Kramer JH, Wyss-Coray C, Gearhart R, et al. Neuropsychological and functional measures of severity in Alzheimer disease, frontotemporal dementia, and semantic dementia. Alzh Dis Assoc Disord 2004; 18: 202–7. Scahill VL, Hodges JR, Graham KS. Can episodic memory tasks differentiate semantic dementia from Alzheimer’s disease? Neurocase 2005; 11: 441–51. Schneider C. Weiter Beitra¨ge zur Lehre von der Pickschen Krankheit [Further contributions to the knowledge of the Pick disease]. Zeitschrift fu¨r die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1929; 120: 340–84. Schofield EC, Hodges JR, Macdonald V, Cordato NJ, Kril JJ, Halliday GM. Cortical atrophy differentiates Richardson’s syndrome from the parkinsonian form of progressive supranuclear palsy. Mov Disord 2011; 26: 256–63. Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957; 20: 11–21. Seeley WW. Selective functional, regional, and neuronal vulnerability in frontotemporal dementia. Curr Opin Neurol 2008; 21: 701–7. Seeley WW, Crawford R, Rascovsky K, Kramer JH, Weiner M, Miller BL, et al. Frontal paralimbic network atrophy in very mild behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 2008; 65: 249–55.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

theories of limbic-neocortical interaction in remote memory. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 2421–9. McKinnon MC, Nica EI, Sengdy P, Kovacevic N, Moscovitch M, Freedman M, et al. Autobiographical memory and patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J Cogn Neurosci 2008; 20: 1839–53. Mion M, Patterson K, Acosta-Cabronero J, Pengas G, IzquierdoGarcia D, Hong YT, et al. What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain 2010; 133: 3256–68. Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Consolidation and the hippocampal complex revisited: In defense of the multiple-trace model. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1998; 8: 297–300. Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Multiple-trace theory and semantic dementia: response to K.S. Graham (1999). Trends Cogn Sci 1999; 3: 87–9. Moss HE, Kopelman MD, Cappelletti M, Davies PdM, Jaldow E. Lost for words or loss of memories? Autobiographical memory in semantic dementia. Cogn Neuropsychol 2003; 20: 703–32. Multhaup KS, Balota DA. Generation effects and source memory in healthy older adults and in adults with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology 1997; 11: 382–91. Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1997; 7: 217–27. Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, Passant U, Stuss D, Black S, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998; 51: 1546–54. Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, Goulding P. Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988; 51: 353–61. Nedjam Z, Devouche E, Dalla Barba G. Confabulation, but not executive dysfunction discriminate AD from frontotemporal dementia. Eur J Neurol 2004; 11: 728–33. Nestor PJ, Fryer TD, Hodges JR. Declarative memory impairments in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neuroimage 2006; 30: 1010–20. Nestor PJ, Fryer TD, Ikeda M, Hodges JR. Retrosplenial cortex (BA 29/ 30) hypometabolism in mild cognitive impairment (prodromal Alzheimer’s disease). Eur J Neurosci 2003; 18: 2663–7. Nestor PJ, Graham KS, Bozeat S, Simons JS, Hodges JR. Memory consolidation and the hippocampus: further evidence from studies of autobiographical memory in semantic dementia and frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 633–54. Neumann M, Rademakers R, Roeber S, Baker M, Kretzschmar HA, Mackenzie IR. A new subtype of frontotemporal lobar degeneration with FUS pathology. Brain 2009; 132: 2922–31. Nichols IC, Weigner WC. Pick’s disease: a specific type of dementia. Brain 1938; 3: 237–49. Pachana NA, Boone KB, Miller BL, Cummings JL, Berman N. Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996; 2: 505–10. Pasquier F, Grymonprez L, Lebert F, van der Linden M. Memory impairment differs in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurocase 2001; 7: 161–71. Pengas G, Patterson K, Arnold RJ, Bird CM, Burgess N, Nestor PJ. Lost and found: bespoke memory testing for Alzheimer’s disease and Semantic Dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2010; 21: 1347–65. Pennington C, Hodges JR, Hornberger M. Neural correlates of episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2011; 24: 261–8. Perri R, Koch G, Carlesimo GA, Serra L, Fadda L, Pasqualetti P, et al. Alzheimer’s disease and frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia–a very brief battery for cognitive and behavioural distinction. J Neurol 2005; 252: 1238–44. Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Differentiating frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2000; 54: 2277–84. Pick A. Uber die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie. Prager Medicinische Wochenschrift 1892; 17: 165–7. Translated by: Girling DM, Berrios GE. On the relationship between senile cerebral atrophy and aphasia. Hist Psychiatry 1994; 5: 542–47.

Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

692

| Brain 2012: 135; 678–692

Urechia CI. Maladie de Pick, diagnostique´e pendant la vie [Diagnosis of Pick’s disease during life]. Eur Neurol 1940; 103: 353–9. van Mansvelt J. Pick’ disease: a syndrome of lobar cerebral atrophy its clinico-anatomical and histopatholigical types. Enschede: J van de Loeff; 1954. Warrington EK. The selective impairment of semantic memory. Q J Exp Psychol 1975; 27: 635–57. Westmacott R, Leach L, Freedman M, Moscovitch M. Different patterns of autobiographical memory loss in semantic dementia and medial temporal lobe amnesia: a challenge to consolidation theory. Neurocase 2001; 7: 37–55. Whitwell JL, Jack CR Jr, Parisi JE, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Petersen RC, et al. Imaging signatures of molecular pathology in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J Mol Neurosci 2011; 45: 372–8. Whitwell JL, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Ivnik RJ, Vemuri P, Gunter JL, et al. Distinct anatomical subtypes of the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: a cluster analysis study. Brain 2009; 132: 2932–46. Wicklund AH, Johnson N, Rademaker A, Weitner BB, Weintraub S. Word list versus story memory in Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal dementia. Alzh Dis Assoc Disord 2006; 20: 86–92. Williams GB, Nestor PJ, Hodges JR. Neural correlates of semantic and behavioural deficits in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage 2005; 24: 1042–51. Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Bontempi B. Memory formation and long-term retention in humans and animals: convergence towards a transformation account of hippocampal-neocortical interactions. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48: 2339–56.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of New South Wales on February 26, 2012

Shrager Y, Gold JJ, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Intact visual perception in memory-impaired patients with medial temporal lobe lesions. J Neurosci 2006; 26: 2235–40. Simons JS, Graham KS, Galton CJ, Patterson K, Hodges JR. Semantic knowledge and episodic memory for faces in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology 2001; 15: 101–14. Simons JS, Verfaellie M, Galton CJ, Miller BL, Hodges JR, Graham KS. Recollection-based memory in frontotemporal dementia: implications for theories of long-term memory. Brain 2002; 125: 2523–36. Simons JS, Verfaellie M, Hodges JR, Lee AC, Graham KS, Koutstaal W, et al. Failing to get the gist: reduced false recognition of semantic associates in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology 2005; 19: 353–61. Snowden JS, Griffiths HL, Neary D. Progressive language disorder associated with frontal lobe degeneration. Neurocase 1996; 2: 429–40. Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 2189–208. Swainson R, Hodges JR, Galton CJ, Semple J, Michael A, Dunn BD, et al. Early detection and differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s Disease and depression with neuropsychological tasks. Dem Geriatr Cogn Disord 2001; 12: 265–80. Thomas-Anterion C, Jacquin K, Laurent B. Differential mechanisms of impairment of remote memory in Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal dementia. Dem Geriatr Cogn Disord 2000; 11: 100–6. Tissot R, Constantinidis J, Richard J. La maladie de Pick. Paris: Masson; 1975.

M. Hornberger and O. Piguet

Lihat lebih banyak...

Comentários

Copyright © 2017 DADOSPDF Inc.